Oncotarget

Research Papers:

Cognition, serum BDNF levels, and BDNF Val66Met polymorphism in type 2 diabetes patients and healthy controls

PDF |  HTML  |  How to cite

Oncotarget. 2018; 9:3653-3662. https://doi.org/10.18632/oncotarget.23342

Metrics: PDF 1408 views  |   HTML 2316 views  |   ?  

Yan-Feng Zhen, Xing-Yu Liu, Dong-Hao Zhou, Xiangdong Du, Guangzhong Yin, Yingyang Zhang, Hui Fang _, Gang Xu, Jair C. Soares and Xiang Yang Zhang

Abstract

Yan-Feng Zhen1,*, Xing-Yu Liu2,*, Dong-Hao Zhou3,*, Xiangdong Du4, Guangzhong Yin4, Yingyang Zhang4, Hui Fang1, Gang Xu5, Jair C. Soares6 and Xiang Yang Zhang6

1Department of Endocrinology, Tangshan Gongren Hospital, Tangshan, China

2Department of Neurosurgery, Tangshan Gongren Hospital, Tangshan, China

3Department of Endocrinology, Linyi People’s Hospital, Linyi, China

4Suzhou Psychiatric Hospital, The Affiliated Guangji Hospital of Soochow University, Suzhou, China

5Department of Burn, Tangshan Gongren Hospital, Tangshan, China

6Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, TX, USA

*These authors contributed equally to this work

Correspondence to:

Hui Fang, email: [email protected]

Xiang Yang Zhang, email: [email protected]

Keywords: type 2 diabetes; brain-derived neurotrophic factor; cognition; polymorphism; association

Received: August 05, 2017     Accepted: November 23, 2017     Published: December 16, 2017

ABSTRACT

Background and aims: Type 2 diabetes (T2DM) is associated with cognitive deficits. However, their pathophysiological mechanisms are still unknown. Recent study suggests that brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is correlated with cognitive deficits in T2DM patients. This study was to determine whether altered serum BDNF levels and cognitive deficits depended on the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism in T2DM.

Results: The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism may not contribute directly to the susceptibility to T2DM. The total and nearly all index scores (all p < 0.01) except for the attention and visuospatial/constructional indexes (both p > 0.05) of RBANS were markedly decreased in T2DM compared with healthy controls. Serum BDNF levels were significantly lower in patients than that in controls (p < 0.001), and BDNF was positively associated with delayed memory in patients (p < 0.05). The Met variant was associated with worse delayed memory performance among T2DM patients but not among normal controls. Moreover, serum BDNF was positively associated with delayed memory among Met homozygote patients (β = 0.29, t = 2.21, p = 0.033), while serum BDNF was negatively associated the RBANS total score (β = –0.92, t = –3.40, p = 0.002) and language index (β = −1.17, t = –3.54, p = 0.001) among Val homozygote T2DM patients.

Conclusions: BDNF gene Val66Met variation may be associated with cognitive deficits in T2DM, especially with delayed memory. The association between lower BDNF serum levels and cognitive impairment in T2DM is dependent on the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism.

Methods: We recruited 311 T2DM patients and 346 healthy controls and compared them on the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS), serum BDNF levels, and the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism.


INTRODUCTION

Type 2 diabetes (T2DM) is often accompanied with cognitive dysfunction, and has an increased rate for developing dementia [1] and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [24]. Previous studies have reported that T2DM was associated with cognitive deficits in certain domains, including immediate and delayed memory, processing speed, learning, as well as executive function [57]. Yet, the exact mechanisms underlying these decrements of cognitive function in T2DM are still unclear.

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is a member of the neurotrophin family that modulates synaptic transmission [8] and hippocampal neuroplasticity that is correlated with learning and memory [9]. More specifically, BDNF induces long-term potentiation (LTP), which is thought to underlie learning and memory [10]. In BDNF knockout mice, hippocampal LTP is significantly decreased, but reversed by administrating exogenous BDNF or increasing BDNF expression [8]. Furthermore, using gene knockout or antisense RNA to inhibit the production of endogenous BDNF resulted in impairment in spatial learning and memory [11], suggesting the critical role of BDNF in learning and memory in the hippocampus [12]. Indeed, previous studies showed that a functional polymorphism Val66Met in the ‘pro’ region of BDNF was associated with a reduction in hippocampal volume [13] and hippocampal-dependent memory in healthy human subjects [9, 12, 13]. Moreover, some studies found that this polymorphism could influence vulnerability of the brain structural network [14] and human memory-related hippocampal activity [15] in non-diabetic populations. For example, a previous study showed that individuals with Met alleles displayed episodic memory deficiencies [9]. A subsequent study also showed that the Met allele correlated with poor medial temporal lobe-related memory performance [16]. Our study also demonstrated a correlation between the Met allele and cognitive deficit in visuospatial/ constructional index in both schizophrenia subjects and healthy controls [17]. Interestingly, Met homozygotes had volume deficits in gray matter, such as frontal, temporal, and thalamus areas in cognitive-declined diseases such as AD and depression [1820]. Val-allele carriers showed better integrities of fiber tracts in white matter compared to Met homozygotes in the occipital area as well as in frontal temporal lobe [21]. Therefore, we hypothesized that BDNF Met-allele carriers would present poorer cognition compared to Val homozygotes in patients with T2DM. However, to the best of our knowledge, the association between Val66Met polymorphism of BDNF and the cognitive performance in T2DM has not been investigated.

To date, several studies have showed the alterations of circulating BDNF in T2DM patients, with mixed results. For example, some studies found decreased BDNF levels in T2DM [2225]; however, the opposite results were also reported in newly diagnosed T2DM patients [26, 27]. The inconsistent results could be attributed to several factors, such as sampling of patients with different complications, different illness courses and clinical profiles, exposure for different medications, or the biological heterogeneity [9, 22]. Hence, the peripheral levels of BDNF merit further investigation in T2DM patients.

Previous research indicates that serum BDNF levels are positively associated with memory and general cognitive performance in healthy seniors [28]. This is also supported by a significantly lower level of BDNF in individuals with cognitive decline-associated disorders, such as AD and mild cognitive impairment [29]. Our previous study found both cognitive impairment and decreased BDNF serum levels in T2DM patients, as well as a positive correlation between delayed memory and BDNF levels in these patients [30]. Interestingly, our finding was confirmed by a subsequent study showing significant association between cognitive impairment and low BDNF levels in T2DM patients [31], suggesting that BDNF plays an important role in cognitive impairment in T2DM patients.

However, none have explored the inter-relationships among cognitive function, BDNF serum levels and BDNF genotype in T2DM patients versus healthy controls. Thus, the purposes of this study were to explore the inter-relationships among cognitive function, BDNF serum levels and BDNF Val66Met gene polymorphism in T2DM as well as healthy subjects. Our main aims were: (1) to confirm the association between peripheral BDNF levels and cognitive performance in both T2DM and healthy subjects; (2) to examine whether cognitive performance may be different in the BDNF genotype subgroups; and (3) to investigate whether the BDNF genotype impacts the relationship of cognitive performance and BDNF serum levels.

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the clinical and demographic data. The patients’ average age was 54.9 ± 10.7 years, and their illness duration was 6.0 ± 1.0 years. BMI, serum TC, fasting glucose and TG were all greater in T2DM patients than those in the healthy subjects (all p < 0.01).

Table 1: Demographic characteristics and BDNF allele and genotype distributions in the controls and patients with type 2 diabetes

Type 2 diabetes patients (n = 311)

Normal controls (n = 346)

Statistics (p value)

Sex (M/F)

136/175

138/208

0.99 (0.32)

Age (years)

54.93 ± 10.73

53.43 ± 9.86

3.5 (0.06)

Education (years)

9.82 ± 3.49

10.01 ± 5.85

0.21 (0.65)

BMI(kg/m2)

26.02 ± 3.73

24.86 ± 4.60

12.0 (<0.01)

WHR

0.92 ± 0.25

0.87 ± 0.07

8.6 (<0.01)

Fasting glucose (mmmol/l)

8.76 ± 2.96

5.02 ± 1.42

405.7 (<0.01)

Lipids (mmmol/l)

 Total cholesterol

5.77 ± 2.02

4.94 ± 1.16

38.7 (<0.01)

 Triglyceride

4.13 ± 4.92

1.38 ± 0.96

97.0 (<0.01)

Duration of illness (years)

6.00 ± 1.10

NA

HbA1c (%)(mmol/mol)

7.42 ± 2.02

NA

Serum BDNF level (ng/ml)

7.73 ± 2.95

11.56 ± 2.67

295.2 (<0.01)

Allele frequency

 Val

51.0%

51.9%

0.11 (0.74)

 Met

49.0%

48.1%

Genotype frequency

 Val/Val

76 (24.4%)

88 (25.4 %)

0.12 (0.94)

 Val/Met

165 (53.1%)

183 (52.9%)

 Met/ Met

70 (22.5%)

75 (21.7%)

Abbreviation: BMI body mass index, WHR Waist-to-hip ratio, HbA1c Hemoglobin A1C.

Both the patients and healthy controls’ genotype distributions were in HWE (both p > 0.05). No significant differences in the BDNF genotype and allele frequencies were observed between the patient and control subjects (Χ2 = 0.12, df = 2, p > 0.05 and Χ2 = 0.11, df = 1, p > 0.05, respectively) (Table 1). In addition, there was no significant differences in genotype distributions between obese and non-obese groups (p > 0.05).

BDNF serum levels in the patient and control subjects

BDNF levels were significantly decreased in T2DM patients than that in healthy subjects (7.7 ± 3.0 vs. 11.6 ± 2.7 ng/ml, F = 295.2, df = 1, 638, p < 0.001) (Table 1). After controlling for gender, age, education, BMI, fasting glucose, TC and TG, BDNF levels were still lower in patients than that in healthy subjects (F = 29.1, df = 8, 537, p < 0.001).

Further, BDNF levels were negatively associated with HbA1c (r = –0.13, df = 264, p = 0.029) in T2DM patients, and inversely with age (r = –0.13, df = 332, p < 0.05) in controls. In addition, there was no significant relationship between BDNF levels and any other clinical variables in either T2DM patients or controls (all p > 0.05).

Cognitive function in the patient and control subjects

Cognitive test by the RBANS were available for 311 patients and 346 healthy controls (Table 2). RBANS total score and its three domain indexes, immediate memory, language, delayed memory were significantly lower in patients than in healthy controls (all p < 0.001). These significant differences still existed after controlling for gender, age, education, BMI, fasting glucose, TC and TG in the ANOVA as covariates (Table 2). Also, these significant differences passed Bonferroni corrections.

Table 2: Total and index scores on the RBANS in patients with type 2 diabetes and normal controls

Type 2 diabetes (n = 311)

Controls (n = 346)

F

P

Adjusted F

P

Immediate memory

69.45 ± 14.46

76.70 ± 17.64

32.7

<0.001

11.8

<0.01

Visuospatial/constructional

81.83 ± 16.06

81.98 ± 15.41

0.02

>0.05

0.01

>0.05

Language

89.05 ± 13.50

94.25 ± 13.03

25.2

<0.001

14.2

<0.001

Attention

85.74 ± 16.73

88.17 ± 20.26

2.8

>0.05

0.08

>0.05

Delayed memory

78.56 ± 13.56

87.84 ± 14.72

70.0

<0.001

17.5

<0.001

Total

75.50 ± 13.42

81.45 ± 15.00

28.4

<0.001

9.4

<0.01

Note: Adjusted F shows the F value controlled for sex, age, education, BMI, TC, TG and fasting glucose.

Association of BDNF with cognitive performance

Serum BDNF levels were positively correlated with the RBANS total score (r = 0.13, df = 306, p < 0.05) and delayed memory index score (r = 0.22, df = 306, p < 0.001) in T2DM patients. However, there was no significant correlation of BDNF with any index or RBANS total scores in the controls (all p > 0.05).

For the controls, multiple regression analysis showed that BDNF did not contribute to any RBANS test scores (all p > 0.05). For the patients, multiple regression analysis demonstrated that age (β = −0.23, t = −3.71, p < 0.001), education (β = 0.48, t = 8.04, p < 0.001), and BDNF (β = 0.12, t = 2.04, p = 0.043) contributed to the RBANS total score. Furthermore, education (β = 0.30, t = 4.55, p < 0.001) and BDNF (β = 0.21, t = 3.26, p < 0.01) were associated with the RBANS delayed memory.

Effects of BDNF genotype on serum in patients and controls

Table 3 showed that the T2DM patients had significantly lower BDNF levels than healthy subjects when sub-grouped by BDNF genotype (all p < 0.01). However, BDNF levels were not significantly different among the three BDNF genotype subgroups in either patients or healthy subjects (both p > 0.05), without genotype × diagnosis effect (p > 0.05), suggesting that the BDNF genotype does not adjust the BDNF levels in both the patient and healthy subjects. After controlling for gender, age, education, BMI, fasting glucose, TC and TG, we still did not observe significant differences in BDNF levels among the three genotypic subgroups in these two groups (both p > 0.05).

Table 3: Serum BDNF levels (ng/ml) in controls and type 2 diabetes by genotype groupings

Val/Val

Met/Val

Met/Met

Controls (n = 332)

11.5 ± 2.4

11.5 ± 2.9

11.8 ± 2.4

Type 2 diabetes (n = 306)

8.2 ± 3.0

7.6 ± 3.0

7.5 ± 2.9

p valuea

<0.01

<0.01

<0.01

aIndicates the comparison between patients and controls by genotype groupings. There was no significant effect of genotype on serum BDNF levels (p > 0.05) or genotype × diagnosis effect (p > 0.05).

Effects of BDNF genotypes on cognitive performance in patients and controls

Cognitive test by RBANS were performed in 311 patients and 346 healthy controls. Table 4 showed their RBANS total and index scores, and the effects of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on the RBANS total and index scores.

Table 4: Comparisons of total and index scores on the RBANS by diagnostic and genotype groupings

Cognitive index

Type 2 diabetes

Controls

Genotype, F (p value)

Genotype× diagnosis, F (p value)

Val/Val
(n = 76)

Met/Val
(n = 165)

Met/Met
(n = 70)

Val/Val
(n = 88)

Met/Val
(n = 183)

Met/Met
(n = 75)

Immediate memory

72.2 ± 14.2

68.9 ± 14.4

67.9 ± 14.9

79.2 ± 16.9

75.3 ± 18.2

77.1 ± 17.1

2.9 (0.06)

0.4 (0.7)

Visuospatial/constructional

80.5 ± 16.3

82.4 ± 16.1

82.0 ± 15.9

82.1 ± 14.9

81.3 ± 15.4

83.5 ± 16.1

0.3 (0.7)

0.6 (0.6)

Language

91.2 ± 13.0

88.5 ± 12.8

88.0 ± 15.4

95.8 ± 12.2

94.3 ± 13.7

92.3 ± 12.3

2.6 (0.07)

0.2 (0.8)

Attention

87.5 ± 15.4

85.9 ± 17.3

83.5 ± 16.8

91.1 ± 18.6

87.2 ± 21.0

87.1 ± 20.2

2.1 (0.13)

0.3 (0.7)

Delayed memory

82.0 ± 14.1

77.7 ± 13.1

76.8 ± 13.5

89.4 ± 14.5

87.5 ± 15.0

86.8 ± 14.5

3.4 (0.04) a

0.5 (0.6)

Total

77.5 ± 12.7

75.3 ± 13.2

73.9 ± 14.5

83.4 ± 13.4

80.7 ± 15.5

80.9 ± 15.5

2.2 (0.11)

0.2 (0.9)

aThere were significant genotype effects on the delayed memory index score (F = 3.4, p < 0.05). In addition, there was also a linear negative correlation between the number of Met 66 alleles and the language, immediate and delayed memory indexes and RBANS total score in patients (all p < 0.05). There was no significant effect of genotype×diagnosis on RBANS total score and all the five subtest indexes (all p > 0.05).

As shown in Table 4, the delayed memory score was significantly different among the three genotypes (F = 3.4, p = 0.03) in T2DM patients. The Met/Met group displayed significantly lower delayed memory score than Va/Val group (p < 0.05). However, no significant difference was observed in the RBANS total and index scores among the three genotype groups in the controls (all p > 0.05) (data not shown).

Further, there was a linear negative association of the number of Met 66 alleles (dummy number, 0, 1, or 2) with the language (β = –0.14, t = –2.23, p < 0.05), immediate (β = –0.26, t = −4.17, p < 0.001) and delayed memory (β = –0.26, t = –4.01, p < 0.001) and the RBANS total scores (β = –0.17, t = –2.90, p < 0.01) in the T2DM patients.

BDNF genotypic effects on associations of BDNF serum levels with cognitive functioning

Regression analyses found a significantly positive association between BDNF levels and delayed memory score (β = 0.29, t = 2.21, p = 0.033) in Met homozygote patients, as well as negative associations between BDNF and RBANS total score (β = –0.92, t = –3.40, p = 0.002) and language index (β = –1.17, t = –3.54, p = 0.001). However, among Val/Met heterozygous patients, no significant associations were found between the BDNF levels and any RBANS scores (all p > 0.05).

DISCUSSION

In our current study, there was no significant difference in the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism between T2DM and the controls, which is consistent with a study in a Caucasian population in Denmark [22] and in a Chinese population [24], suggesting that the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism may be not associated with susceptibility to T2DM directly. Interestingly, the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism has been studied for possible association with obesity, a key pathogenic factor in development of T2DM, but reports are contradictory [3235]. In our present, we did not find correlation of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism with obesity in a Chinese Han population. One possible reason for this inconsistent result may be associated with different ethnicities with variable allele frequency distribution of BDNF Val66Met. For example, in our study, the frequency of the Met allele was 48.1% in the controls, which is close to other reports in Chinese population [24], but different from other populations, such as Caucasian subjects [22]. Therefore, the differences in the BDNF Val66Met genotype distribution frequencies in different ethnicities may result in inconsistency across the studies from the different populations. In addition, it is worth noting that a SNP (rs4074134) near the BDNF gene was recently reported to be associated with T2DM independently of obesity in the Chinese Han population [36]. Hence, the role of the BDNF polymorphisms and haplotypes in the development of T2DM deserves further examination in different populations.

BDNF Val66Met variant and cognitive impairments in T2DM patients

The RBANS total score and three index scores- immediate memory, language and delayed memory, were significantly lower in T2DM patients than in healthy subjects. This supports the notion that T2DM has decrements in many aspects of cognitive function [1, 57], and increases the risk for some diseases associated with cognitive decline, such as AD and vascular dementia [24].

We found that delayed memory index score were significantly lower in the BDNF-Met homozygous carriers than in the Val allele carriers in T2DM patients. Further regression analysis found that the number of Met66 alleles was positively correlated with the performance on delayed memory in T2DM patients, supporting that Met-allele may contribute to impaired memory function [9, 12]. However, the exact mechanisms underlying the impact of the Met allele on these cognitive domains are still unclear. Previous reports demonstrated that the BDNF Val66Met variant was associated with reductions in hippocampal-dependent memory and in hippocampal volume [12, 13] in control subjects. Moreover, Pezawas et al. [37] found that BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affected the function of BDNF in neurons, and correlated with several neurological and psychiatric disorders, especially the alteration in human memory. Preclinical studies showed that rats transfected with the BDNF-Met allele exhibited a lower secretion of BDNF than those transfected with the Val allele [38] in hippocampal neurons. Accordingly, the BDNF-Met variant affects memory and hippocampal function by reducing BDNF transmission within cells and activity-dependent secretion in humans [9]. Furthermore, Met-homozygous carriers show poorer memory function than their Val allele counterparts in humans [9]. In short, these studies indicated that the BDNF Met-66 variant may influence memory in humans, with or without T2DM.

Cognitive deficits and low BDNF serum: association with BDNF Val66Met genotype

The present study demonstrated that lower BDNF levels were positively associated with cognitive performance in T2DM patients, consistent with recent studies demonstrating association of BDNF levels with cognitive function in ageing adults [28]. However, the underlying mechanisms for these results are still unclear. One possible reason is that BDNF has neuroprotective effect, which is involved in regulating synaptic transmission [8] and activity-dependent neuroplasticity that is critical for learning and memory in the hippocampus [9]. It can also induce long-term potentiation (LTP), which is regarded as the neurophysiological mechanisms for learning and memory [10]. Preclinical studies show that hippocampal LTP was markedly reduced in BDNF knockout mice, and could be reversed by administrating exogenous BDNF or increasing BDNF expression [9]. Taken together, these findings suggest a close correlation between cognitive function and BDNF level.

However, the origin of peripheral BDNF are still unclear. Although BDNF is mainly expressed in central nervous system, it also occurs in the circulatory system, such as in serum and platelets [39, 40]. Studies have demonstrated that BDNF can pass the blood-brain barrier [39], and serum BDNF levels were found to be associated with that in the brain [40], suggesting that peripheral BDNF may reflect its levels in the brain. Hence, our finding of decreased BDNF may reflect low BDNF brain levels, which could impact cognitive performance in T2DM patients. Indeed, we found that BDNF levels were positively related with delayed memory score in T2DM patients with Met/Met homozygote, while BDNF levels were negatively correlated with the RBANS total and language scores in those with Val/Val homozygote. However, the clinical significance of these discrepant BDNF-cognitive function associations in different BDNF genotypes in T2DM patients are still unknown.

Like other studies, we found decreased BDNF levels in T2DM patients [23, 24], which were not associated with the BDNF Val66Met genotype in both patients and healthy subjects. However, a recent study found that the Val66Met genotype affected serum BDNF levels, showing that Met-allele carriers had higher levels of BDNF in European populations [41]. Hence the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism may correlate with BDNF serum levels in some western population, but not in Asian population. More specifically, a previous study reported that the Met variant was associated with decrease only in the activity-dependent BDNF secretion [17]. The relationship between peripheral BDNF and BDNF gene polymorphisms warrants further investigation.

However, there were several limitations in this study. First, previous studies reported that exercise [42], smoking [43] and alcohol [44] were correlated with cognition. However, unfortunately, we did not collect the data for exercise level, smoking or alcohol drinking in our current study, which will be remedied in future investigation. Second, that Val66Met polymorphism may be related to delayed memory is actually a very important observation in our current study. We should provide another way to double confirm this important finding, such as using MRI test, since some previous studies have shown that Met allele affect human memory-related hippocampal activity in healthy persons [15]. Also Met homozygotes showed decreased gray matter volume [1820] and worse integrities of fiber tracts in white matter compared to Val allele carriers [21]. Unfortunately, we did not carry out the MRI test in our current study. However, we are performing the MRI test in our ongoing project. Third, our T2DM patients had old age of average 54.9 years and longer duration of illness of average 6 years and had been on oral hypoglycemic, which limit the generalization of our findings to other studies. Fourth, BDNF levels were measured in serum, but not in central nervous system. It remains unknown whether there is a parallel alteration between peripheral BDNF and the central nervous system.

In summary, this study has provided new evidence to support decreased BDNF serum level in T2DM patients, which was significantly associated with the degree of cognitive impairments in T2DM, suggesting that circulating BDNF level may be considered as biomarker of cognitive function in T2DM patients. Also, we found the BDNF-Met allele carrier exhibited a poorer delayed memory index score than their Val counterparts in T2DM patients, suggesting that the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism was involved in some domains of cognitive deficits in T2DM patients but not in healthy subjects. Furthermore, the association of serum BDNF level with cognitive impairment in T2DM patients was moderated by the BDNF-Val66Met polymorphism. Our study, however, is limited by its moderate sample size, wide age range from 20–70 years and longer duration of illness. Additionally, it should be mentioned that that there are significant differences in allele frequency of BDNF Val66Met varies between Asian and Caucasian populations, showing that the frequency of Met allele was 48.1% in our present study, which is almost 2.5 times more than that in Caucasian subjects (around 20%) [38]. Thus, it is likely that the specific role of the BDNF Met allele in cognitive deficits, especially delayed memory in T2DM patients may be limited to Chinese or Asian populations, which may not be adapted to the western patients. Hence, the association between cognitive impairment and the BDNF Met variant in T2DM will need to be confirmed in the future studies in different ethnicities, for instance, in Caucasian population.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Three hundred and eleven outpatients (male/female = 136/175) were recruited from the Tangshan Gongren Hospital in TangShan city, 50 miles from Beijing in the period from March 2008 to March 2010. Inclusion criteria included: (1) aged between 20–70 years, Han Chinese; (2) meeting the diagnosis of T2DM according to the World Health Organization 1999 criteria [45]; (3) illness duration < 10 years; (4) without any history of diagnosed coronary artery disease, cerebrovascular disease, stroke, known central nervous system or neuropsychiatric diseases, and any other complications (nephropathy or retinopathy) of diabetes [46, 47]; (5) without any audiovisual or motor coordination impairment affecting the cognitive function tests; (6) be able to complete neurocognitive test. In addition, diabetic nephropathy was defined by increased urinary albumin excretion (UAE) in the absence of other renal diseases, with UAE >20 μg/min [48]. Diabetic retinopathy was defined as the presence of typical retinal microvascular lesions in an individual with diabetes. High-quality fundus photographs of both eyes from all patients were taken and interpreted by a retinal specialist using the International Clinical Diabetic Retinopathy and Diabetic Macular Edema Disease Severity Scales [49]. Diabetic retinopathy was deemed to be present if characteristic lesions were detected (i.e. microaneurisms, hemorrhages, cotton wool spots, intraretinal microvascular abnormalities, hard exudates, and new retinal vessels). All of the patients were receiving conventional medical treatment with the most commonly prescribed drugs being oral hypoglycemics such as Metformin and Repaglinide.

Three hundred and forty-six normal controls (male/female = 138/208) were recruited from the community in Beijing at the same period in parallel with T2DM patients, and matched for sex, age, and education. All controls were in good physical and mental health.

We obtained a complete medical history and conducted physical examinations and laboratory tests for both patients and control subjects. Subjects with any other illnesses, or drug or alcohol abuse/dependence were excluded from this study. All subjects were Han Chinese. They gave signed and informed consent to participate in the study, which was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Tangshan Gongren Hospital.

Clinical measures

General information, socio-demographic characteristics, medical and psychological conditions of all subjects were collected by a member of the research staff. Additional information was collected from available medical records.

Cognitive assessment

We individually measured cognitive functioning of all subjects using the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS, Form A) [50]. The RBANS was previously translated into Chinese by our group and its clinical validity and test-retest reliability established among controls and schizophrenia patients [51]. The subjects were administered with the RBANS test on the same day as the blood sample withdrawal. All the subjects performed the cognitive test at least 1 h after breakfast or lunch. No one had taken the cognitive test after an overnight fast.

Four researchers rated subjects on this scale after simultaneously attending a training session in using the RBANS. After training, repeated assessment indicated that the four raters maintained a correlation coefficient for the RBANS total score greater than 0.8. All the four researchers were blinded to all subjects.

Blood sampling and serum BDNF measurements

We collected serum samples at the same period from patients and normal controls between 7 and 9 a.m. following an overnight fast. The serum was separated, aliquoted, and stored at −70°C before use.

We applied a commercially available, sandwich enzyme linked immunosorbent assay to measure serum BDNF levels, as previously described [52]. All samples were assayed by a research assistant blind to the clinical status. Inter- and intra-assay variation coefficients were 8 and 5%, respectively.

Genotyping

The genotypes of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism were identified as reported previously [53]. A research assistant who was blind to the clinical status genotyped every subject twice to ensure accuracy.

Statistical analysis

Deviations from the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) were assessed using the x2 goodness-of-fit test. Chi-squared tests were used to compare BDNF Val66Met allele and genotype frequencies between T2DM patients and healthy controls. Demographic and clinical variables of the T2DM patients and healthy controls were compared using analysis of variance (ANOVA) for the continuous variables, and chi-squared for the categorical variables. Since the BDNF variables were normally distributed in the patients and normal controls (Kolmogorov-mirnov one-sample test; both p > 0.05), the principal outcome analysis consisted of one-way ANOVA. When ANOVA was significant, the effect of sex, age, education, body mass index (BMI), and the clinical variables were tested by adding these variables to the analysis model as covariates. Relationships between variables were assessed with Pearson’s product moment correlation coefficients.

For the main models, the BDNF genotype and diagnosis (cases vs controls) were entered as fixed effects. Scores for each cognitive domain and the total scores of RBANS were entered as the dependent variables, with sex, age, and education included as covariates as appropriate. In each model, the main effect of diagnostic group, the main effect of genotype, and diagnostic group × genotype interaction were tested. The diagnostic group × genotype interaction term in the model detects the differential effects that alleles might have on cognitive scores between diagnostic groups. Similarly, the main effect of the BDNF genotype on serum BDNF levels was also analyzed using ANCOVA. Bonferroni corrections were applied to each test to adjust for multiple testing.

Lastly, we performed exploratory regression analyses to examine whether the relationships between BDNF serum levels and cognitive function were different across BDNF genotype groups. Stepwise multiple regression analysis used RBANS total or Index scores as dependent variables, with BDNF levels as the independent variable in each BDBF genotype group. Covariates in these stepwise forward entry models included age, sex, education, BMI, serum total cholesterol (TC) and triacylglycerol (TG), fasting glucose in both the patient and control groups, and clinical variables in the patient group, such as duration of illness and hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c). SPSS version 16.0 was used for all of the statistical analyses. Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

None.

FUNDING

Funding for this study was provided by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81371477), the NARSAD Independent Investigator Grant (20314) and the Natural Science Foundation of Hebei Province (H2015105083).

REFERENCES

1. Ryan CM, van Duinkerken E, Rosano C. Neurocognitive consequences of diabetes. Am Psychol. 2016; 71:563–576.

2. Ravona-Springer R, Luo X, Schmeidler J, Wysocki M, Lesser G, Rapp M, Dahlman K, Grossman H, Haroutunian V, Schnaider Beeri M. Diabetes is associated with increased rate of cognitive decline in questionably demented elderly. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010; 29:68–74.

3. Kravitz E, Schmeidler J, Schnaider Beeri M. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive compromise: potential roles of diabetes-related therapies. Endocrinol Metab Clin North Am. 2013; 42:489–501.

4. Biessels GJ, Strachan MW, Visseren FL, Kappelle LJ, Whitmer RA. Dementia and cognitive decline in type 2 diabetes and prediabetic stages: towards targeted interventions. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2014; 2:246–255.

5. Saedi E, Gheini MR, Faiz F, Arami MA. Diabetes mellitus and cognitive impairments. World J Diabetes. 2016; 7:412–422.

6. Nooyens AC, Baan CA, Spijkerman AM, Verschuren WM. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline in middle-aged men and women: the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Diabetes Care. 2010; 33:1964–1969.

7. Chin SO, Rhee SY, Chon S, Baik SH, Park Y, Nam MS, Lee KW, Chun KH, Woo JT, Kim YS. Hypoglycemia is associated with dementia in elderly patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus: An analysis based on the Korea National Diabetes Program Cohort. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2016; 122:54-61.

8. Lu Y, Christian K, Lu B. BDNF: A Key Regulator for Protein-synthesis Dependent LTP and Long-term Memory? Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008; 89: 312–323.

9. Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Bertolino A, Zaitsev E, Gold B, Goldman D, Dean M, Lu B, Weinberger DR. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell. 2003; 112:257–269.

10. Guzowski JF, Lyford GL, Stevenson GD, Houston FP, McGaugh JL, Worley PF, Barnes CA. Inhibition of activity-dependent arc protein expression in the rat hippocampus impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation and the consolidation of long-term memory. J Neurosci. 2000; 20:3993–4001.

11. Diogenes MJ, Costenla AR, Lopes LV, Jerónimo-Santos A, Sousa VC, Fontinha BM, Ribeiro JA, Sebastiao AM. Enhancement of LTP in aged rats is dependent on endogenous BDNF. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011; 36:1823–1836.

12. Leal G, Bramham CR, Duarte CB. BDNF and Hippocampal Synaptic Plasticity. Vitam Horm. 2017; 104:153–195.

13. Borba EM, Duarte JA, Bristot G, Scotton E, Camozzato AL, Chaves ML. Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Serum Levels and Hippocampal Volume in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia due to Alzheimer Disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2016; 6:559–567.

14. Park CH, Kim J, Namgung E, Lee DW, Kim GH, Kim M, Kim N, Kim TD, Kim S, Lyoo IK, Yoon S.The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Affects the Vulnerability of the Brain Structural Network. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017; 11:1–10.

15. Hariri AR, Goldberg TE, Mattay VS, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor val66met Polymorphism Affects Human Memory-Related Hippocampal Activity and Predicts Memory Performance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003; 23:6690–6694.

16. Ho BC, Milev P, O’Leary DS, Librant A, Andreasen NC, Wassink TH. Cognitive and magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometric correlates of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met gene polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia and healthy volunteers. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006; 63:731–740.

17. Zhang XY, Chen DC, Xiu MH, Haile CN, Luo X, Xu K, Zhang HP, Zuo L, Zhang Z, Zhang X, Kosten TA, Kosten TR. Cognitive and serum BDNF correlates of BDNF Val66Met gene polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls. Hum Genet. 2012; 131:1187–1195.

18. Montag C, Weber B, Fliessbach K, Elger C, Reuter M. The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impacts parahippocampal and amygdala volume in healthy humans: incremental support for a genetic risk factor for depression. Psychol Med. 2009; 39:1831–1839.

19. Forde NJ, Ronan L, Suckling J, Scanlon C, Neary S, Holleran L, Leemans A, Tait R, Rua C, Fletcher PC, Jeurissen B, Dodds CM, Miller SR, et al. Structural neuroimaging correlates of allelicvariation of the BDNF val66met polymorphism. Neuroimage. 2014; 90:280–289.

20. Chiang MC, Barysheva M, Toga AW, Medland SE, Hansell NK, James MR, McMahon KL, de Zubicaray GI, Martin NG, Wright MJ, Thompson PM. BDNF gene effects on brain circuitry replicated in 455 twins. Neuroimage. 2011; 55:448–454.

21. Tost H, Alam T, Geramita M, Rebsch C, Kolachana B, Dickinson D, Verchinski BA, Lemaitre H, Barnett AS, Trampush JW, Weinberger DR, Marenco S. Effects of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphismon white-matter micro structure in healthy adults. Neuropsycho­pharmacology. 2013; 38:525–532.

22. Krabbe KS, Nielsen AR, Krogh-Madsen R, Plomgaard P, Rasmussen P, Erikstrup C, Fischer CP, Lindegaard B, Petersen AM, Taudorf S, Secher NH, Pilegaard H, Brunsgaard H, Pedersen BK. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia. 2007; 50:431–438.

23. Fujinami A, Ohta K, Obayashi H, Fukui M, Hasegawa G, Nakamura N, Kozai H, Imai S, Ohta M. Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor in patients with type 2 Diabetes mellitus: Relationship to glucose metabolism and biomarkers of insulin resistance. Clin Biochem. 2008; 41:812–817.

24. Liu W, Han X, Zhou X, Zhang S, Cai X, Zhang L, Li Y, Li M, Gong S, Ji L. Brain derived neurotrophic factor in newly diagnosed diabetes and prediabetes. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2016; 429:106–113.

25. Li B, Lang N, Cheng ZF. Serum Levels of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Are Associated with Diabetes Risk, Complications, and Obesity: a Cohort Study from Chinese Patients with Type 2 Diabetes. Mol Neurobiol. 2016; 53:5492–5499.

26. Suwa M, Kishimoto H, Nofuji Y, Nakano H, Sasaki H, Radak Z, Kumagai S. Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor level is increased and associated with obesity in newly diagnosed female patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Metabolism. 2006; 55:852–857.

27. Agilli M, Aydin FN, Kurt YG, Cayci T. Evaluation of serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels in type 2 diabetes mellitus patients with and without depressive symptoms. Acta Biochim Biophys Sin. 2015; 47:313.

28. Gunstad J, Benitez A, Smith J, Glickman E, Spitznagel MB, Alexander T, Juvancic-Heltzel J, Murray L. Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor is associated with cognitive function in healthy older adults. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2008; 21:166–170.

29. Kim BY, Lee SH, Graham PL, Angelucci F, Lucia A, Pareja-Galeano H, Leyhe T, Turana Y, Lee IR, Yoon JH, Shin JI. Peripheral Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Levels in Alzheimer’s Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment: a Comprehensive Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Mol Neurobiol. 2016 Nov 4. [Epub ahead of print].

30. Zhen YF, Zhang J, Liu XY, Fang H, Tian LB, Zhou DH, Kosten TR, Zhang XY. Low BDNF is associated with cognitive deficits in patients with type 2 diabetes. Psychopharmacology. 2013; 227:93–100.

31. Murillo Ortiz B, Ramirez Emiliano J, Ramos-Rodriguez E, Martinez-Garza S, Macias-Cervantes H, Solorio-Meza S, Pereyra-Nobara TA. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor plasma levels and premature cognitive impairment/dementia in type 2diabetes. World J Diabetes. 2016; 7:615–620.

32. Thorleifsson G, Walters GB, Gudbjartsson DF, Steinthorsdottir V, Sulem P, Helgadottir A, Styrkarsdottir U. Genome-wide association yields new sequence variants at seven loci that associate with measures of obesity. Nat Genet. 2009; 41:18–24.

33. Lango AH, Estrada K, Lettre G, Berndt SI, Weedon MN, Rivadeneira F, Willer CJ, Jackson AU. Hundreds of variants clustered in genomic loci and biological pathways affect human height. Nature. 2010; 467:832–838.

34. Fujisawa T, Nakashima E, Ikegami H, Nakamura J, Yamori Y, Yamaguchi S, Kobayashi S, Ogihara T, Takayanagi R, Kato N. Association of genetic variants for susceptibility to obesity with type 2 diabetes in Japanese individuals. Diabetologia. 2011; 54:1350–1359.

35. Kong X, Zhang X, Zhao Q, He J, Chen L, Zhao Z, Li Q, Ge J, Chen G, Guo X, Lu J, Weng J, Jia W, et al. Obesity-related genomic loci are associated with type 2 diabetes in a Han Chinese population. PLoS One. 2014; 9:e104486.

36. Han X, Luo Y, Zhang X, Lv C, Sun X, Zhang X, Zhou X, Cai X, Ren Q, Ji L. Rs4074134 near BDNF gene is associated with type 2 diabetes mellitus in Chinese Han population independently of body mass index. PLoS One. 2013; 8:e56898.

37. Pezawas L, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, Callicott JH, Kolachana BS, Straub RE, Egan MF, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Weinberger DR. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical morphology. J Neurosci. 2004; 24:10099–10102.

38. Chen ZY, Patel PD, Sant G, Meng CX, Teng KK, Hempstead BL, Lee FS. Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Met66) alters the intracellular trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of wild-type BDNF in neurosecretory cells and cortical neurons. J Neurosci. 2004; 24:4401–4411.

39. Pan W, Banks WA, Fasold MB, Bluth J, Kastin AJ. Transport of brain-derived neurotrophic factor across the blood-brain barrier. Neuropharmacology. 1998; 37:1553–1561.

40. Karege F, Schwald M, Cisse M. Postnatal developmental profile of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in rat brain and platelets. Neurosci Lett. 2002; 328:261–264.

41. Lango AH, Estrada K, Lettre G, Berndt SI, Weedon MN, Rivadeneira F, Willer CJ, Jackson AU. Hundreds of variants clustered in genomic loci and biological pathways affect human height. Nature. 2010; 467:832–838.

42. Callisaya ML, Daly RM, Sharman JE, Bruce D, Davis TME, Greenaway T, Nolan M, Beare R, Schultz MG, Phan T, Blizzard LC, Srikanth VK. Feasibility of a multi-modal exercise program on cognition in older adults with Type 2 diabetes - a pilot randomised controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2017; 17:237.

43. Naha N, Gandhi DN, Gautam AK, Prakash JR. Nicotine and cigarette smoke modulate Nrf2-BDNF-dopaminergic signal and neurobehavioral disorders in adult rat cerebral cortex. Hum Exp Toxic. 2017 Jan 1. [Epub ahead of print].

44. Topiwala A, Allan CL, Valkanova V, Zsoldos E, Filippini N, Sexton C, Mahmood A, Fooks P, Singh-Manoux A, Mackay CE, Kivimäki M, Ebmeier KP. Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and cognitive decline: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ. 2017; 357:j2353.

45. Alberti KG, Zimmet PZ. Definition, diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus and its complications. Part 1: diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus provisional report of a WHO consultation. Diabet Med. 1998; 15:539–553.

46. Crosby-Nwaobi R, Sivaprasad S, Forbes A. A systematic review of the Association of diabetic retinopathy and cognitive impairment in people with Type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2012; 96:101–110.

47. Umegaki H, Iimuro S, Shinozaki T, Araki A, Sakurai T, Iijima K, Ohashi Y, Ito H, Japanese Elderly Diabetes Intervention Trial Study Group. Risk factors associated with cognitive decline in the elderly with type 2 diabetes: baseline data analysis of the Japanese Elderly Diabetes Intervention Trial. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2012; 12:103–109.

48. Gross JL, de Azevedo MJ, Silveiro SP, Canani LH, Caramori ML, Zelmanovitz T. Diabetic nephropathy: diagnosis, prevention, and treatment. Diabetes Care. 2005; 28:164–176.

49. Wilkinson CP, Ferris FL, Klein RE, Lee PP, Agardh CD, Davis M, Dills D, Kampik A, Pararajasegaram R, Verdaguer JT; Global Diabetic Retinopathy Project Group. Proposed international clinical diabetic retinopathy and diabetic macular edema disease severity scales. Ophthalmology. 2003; 110:1677–1682.

50. Randolph C, Tierney MC, Mohr E, Chase TN. The repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998; 20:310–319.

51. Zhang BH, Tan YL, Zhang WF, Wang ZR, Yang GG, Shi C, Zhang XY, Zhou DF. Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status (RBANS) as a screening test in Chinese: reliability and validity. Chin Ment Health J. 2009; 28: 865–869.

52. Xiu MH, Hui L, Dang YF, Hou TD, Zhang CX, Zheng YL, da Chen C, Kosten TR, Zhang XY. Decreased serum BDNF levels in chronic institutionalized schizophrenia on long-term treatment with typical and atypical antipsychotics. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009; 33:1508–1512.

53. Zhang XY, Zhou DF, Wu GY, Cao LY, Tan YL, Haile CN, Li J, Lu L, Kosten TA, Kosten TR. BDNF levels and genotype are associated with antipsychotic-induced weight gain in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008; 33:2200–2205.


Creative Commons License All site content, except where otherwise noted, is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License.
PII: 23342