Oncotarget

Research Papers: Gerotarget (Focus on Aging):

Effects of circadian clock genes and environmental factors on cognitive aging in old adults in a Taiwanese population

PDF |  HTML  |  Supplementary Files  |  How to cite

Oncotarget. 2017; 8:24088-24098. https://doi.org/10.18632/oncotarget.15493

Metrics: PDF 1795 views  |   HTML 2615 views  |   ?  

Eugene Lin _, Po-Hsiu Kuo, Yu-Li Liu, Albert C. Yang, Chung-Feng Kao and Shih-Jen Tsai

Abstract

Eugene Lin1,2,3, Po-Hsiu Kuo4, Yu-Li Liu5, Albert C. Yang6,7, Chung-Feng Kao8 and Shih-Jen Tsai6,7

1 Institute of Clinical Medical Science, China Medical University, Taichung, Taiwan

2 Vita Genomics, Inc., Taipei, Taiwan

3 TickleFish Systems Corporation, Seattle, WA, USA

4 Department of Public Health, Institute of Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan

5 Center for Neuropsychiatric Research, National Health Research Institutes, Miaoli County, Taiwan

6 Department of Psychiatry, Taipei Veterans General Hospital, Taipei, Taiwan

7 Division of Psychiatry, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan

8 Department of Agronomy, College of Agriculture & Natural Resources, National Chung Hsing University, Taichung, Taiwan

Correspondence to:

Eugene Lin, email:

Shih-Jen Tsai, email:

Keywords: circadian clock genes; circadian rhythms; cognitive aging; gene-gene and gene-environment interactions; single nucleotide polymorphisms; Gerotarget

Received: September 20, 2016 Accepted: February 06, 2017 Published: February 16, 2017

Abstract

Previous animal studies have indicated associations between circadian clock genes and cognitive impairment . In this study, we assessed whether 11 circadian clockgenes are associated with cognitive aging independently and/or through complex interactions in an old Taiwanese population. We also analyzed the interactions between environmental factors and these genes in influencing cognitive aging. A total of 634 Taiwanese subjects aged over 60 years from the Taiwan Biobank were analyzed. Mini-Mental State Examinations (MMSE) were administered to all subjects, and MMSE scores were used to evaluate cognitive function. Our data showed associations between cognitive aging and single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in 4 key circadian clock genes, CLOCK rs3749473 (p = 0.0017), NPAS2 rs17655330 (p = 0.0013), RORA rs13329238 (p = 0.0009), and RORB rs10781247 (p = 7.9 x 10-5). We also found that interactions between CLOCK rs3749473, NPAS2 rs17655330, RORA rs13329238, and RORB rs10781247 affected cognitive aging (p = 0.007). Finally, we investigated the influence of interactions between CLOCK rs3749473, RORA rs13329238, and RORB rs10781247 with environmental factors such as alcohol consumption, smoking status, physical activity, and social support on cognitive aging (p = 0.002 ~ 0.01). Our study indicates that circadian clock genes such as the CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB genes may contribute to the risk of cognitive aging independently as well as through gene-gene and gene-environment interactions.


Introduction

Circadian rhythms are naturally recurring cycles that influence the timing of biological events such as sleep-wake cycles, hormone release, and energy metabolism [1]. The intracellular molecular machinery underlying circadian rhythms indicates that circadian oscillations are stimulated and maintained by a panel of core circadian clock genes, defined as genes whose protein products are necessary components for generating circadian rhythms within individual cells [2]. Core circadian clock genes include the aryl hydrocarbon receptor nuclear translocator like (ARNTL), clock circadian regulator (CLOCK), cryptochrome circadian clock 1 (CRY1), cryptochrome circadian clock 2 (CRY2), neuronal PAS domain protein 2 (NPAS2), nuclear receptor subfamily 1 group D member 1 (NR1D1), period circadian clock 1 (PER1), period circadian clock 2 (PER2), period circadian clock 3 (PER3), RAR related orphan receptor A (RORA), and RAR related orphan receptor B (RORB) genes [2]. In mammals, the molecular clock mechanism is presently viewed as a complicated interplay of transcriptional feedback regulatory loops involving various circadian clock genes that control and support circadian rhythms [3]. The key circadian clock gene ARNTL encodes the ARNTL protein, which is an elementary helix-loop-helix protein that forms heterodimers with either CLOCK or NPAS2, two other primary helix-loop-helix proteins [2]. The ARNTL/CLOCK heterodimer initiates the transcription of numerous target genes such as PER1, PER2, PER3, CRY1, and CRY2 via E-box elements in the promoters of the target genes [2]. One after another, the resulting PER and CRY proteins inhibit further ARNTL/CLOCK transcriptional activity; a new cycle then proceeds due to the low level of ARNTL/CLOCK transcription to enhance the robustness of the oscillations based on the main loop, an additional feedback loop is formed with nuclear receptors such as NR1D1 (or Rev-erb alpha), RORA, and RORB, which initiate the circadian transcription of the ARNTL gene and thus contribute to the timing of the core clock machinery [3].

Previous works have shown that the circadian clock is involved in regulation of brain cognitive functions such as memory, mood, and adaptation to novelty [4-6]. Furthermore, several animal studies have indicated that impairment of memory and adaptation to novelty can result from a loss-of-function mutation in circadian clock genes, such as Arntl [7], Clock [7], Cry1 [8], Cry2 [8], Npas2 [9], and Per2 [10]. In addition, an age-associated decline in the activity of the circadian clock can contribute to cognitive aging or declines in multiple brain functions, such as sleep, mood, and memory, indicating that disturbed circadian rhythms may affect cognitive functions [5]. Therefore, alterations of the circadian clock gene system may play a causative role in cognitive decline in neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s diseases (AD) [11, 12]. Specifically, several recent association studies have indicated that single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) within the ARNTL and CLOCK genes are associated with AD risk [13-16].

While several encouraging findings on the relationship between circadian clock genes and cognitive aging have emerged, to our knowledge, human data is scarce in terms of SNPs. Moreover, the interplay between circadian clock genes and environmental factors such as alcohol consumption, smoking status, physical activity, and social support, has not been comprehensively evaluated in previous association studies. Given that circadian rhythms and their relevant genes may play a key role in the development of cognitive aging, we hypothesized that core circadian clock genes may contribute to the etiology of cognitive aging independently and/or through complex interactions. The gene panel investigated here comprises the 11 aforementioned core circadian clock genes (Supplementary Table 1), namely the ARNTL, CLOCK, CRY1, CRY2, NPAS2, NR1D1, PER1, PER2, PER3, RORA, and RORB genes.

Results

Table 1 describes the demographic and clinical characteristics of the study population, comprised of 634 subjects. The median MMSE score was 27 and the interquartile range was 25–29.

First, we investigated the association between cognitive aging and the 11 circadian clock genes mentioned above. Among the 644 SNPs assessed in this study (Supplementary Table 1), there were 74 SNPs in 8 of the circadian clock genes that demonstrated evidence of association (P < 0.05) with MMSE scores (Table 2). However, only the association of the RORB rs10781247 SNP with MMSE scores nearly reached significance after applying a Bonferroni correction (P < 0.05/644 = 7.8 x 10-5). As shown in Table 2, the RORB rs10781247 SNP indicated an association with MMSE scores among subjects after adjusting for covariates such as age, gender, and education in genetic models, including the additive model (P = 0.0021) and dominant model (P = 7.9 x 10-5).

Table 1: Demographic and clinical characteristics of study subjects.

Characteristic

Overall

No. of subjects, n

634

Mean age ± SD, years

64.2±2.9

Male, n (%)/Female, n (%)

318 (50.2)/316 (49.8)

Married, n

520

Living alone, n

54

Any physical activity, n

404

Current alcohol drinker, n

35

Current smoker, n

41

High school graduate, n

376

MMSE score, median (IQR)

27 (25–29)

Abbreviations: IQR = interquartile range, MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination, SD = standard deviation.

Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation

Table 2: Linear regression models of associations between the MMSE scores and 8 selective circadian clock genes, which have an evidence of association (P < 0.05).

Additive model

Recessive model

Dominant model

Gene

CHR

SNP

A1

A2

MAF

BETA

SE

P

BETA

SE

P

BETA

SE

P

ARNTL

11

rs4757151

G

A

0.35

0.26

0.18

0.1604

0.22

0.35

0.5268

0.56

0.23

0.0148

CLOCK

4

rs3749473

T

C

0.09

0.90

0.47

0.0561

1.66

0.94

0.0799

0.88

0.28

0.0017

rs11932595

G

A

0.07

1.68

0.81

0.0388

3.27

1.63

0.0455

0.74

0.31

0.0168

rs62303728

A

G

0.07

1.80

1.00

0.0711

3.51

2.00

0.0795

0.65

0.31

0.0358

CRY2

11

rs4756035

T

C

0.46

0.18

0.16

0.2478

0.59

0.27

0.0291

-0.12

0.25

0.6310

NPAS2

2

rs57365275

A

G

0.31

-0.41

0.21

0.0454

-0.76

0.40

0.0544

-0.25

0.22

0.2653

rs59005495

T

C

0.33

-0.32

0.19

0.0895

-0.74

0.36

0.0416

0.03

0.23

0.9033

rs983287

G

A

0.24

0.18

0.24

0.4540

0.16

0.47

0.7383

0.46

0.23

0.0410

rs17024926

T

C

0.41

0.27

0.16

0.0948

0.27

0.29

0.3433

0.48

0.24

0.0481

rs12712084

T

C

0.32

-0.05

0.19

0.7940

-0.45

0.36

0.2059

0.54

0.23

0.0175

rs1369481

T

C

0.25

-0.01

0.23

0.9660

-0.31

0.44

0.4793

0.57

0.22

0.0108

rs17654772

A

G

0.07

-2.53

1.00

0.0116

-5.05

2.00

0.0117

-0.24

0.34

0.4785

rs17655330

A

C

0.08

-3.21

0.99

0.0013

-6.42

1.99

0.0013

-0.05

0.32

0.8860

rs73945847

T

C

0.49

0.27

0.16

0.0920

0.63

0.27

0.0186

0.08

0.26

0.7597

rs4851390

G

A

0.49

0.25

0.16

0.1246

0.59

0.26

0.0242

0.05

0.26

0.8350

rs12622050

G

A

0.31

-0.45

0.20

0.0282

-0.96

0.39

0.0138

-0.04

0.23

0.8766

rs4851391

C

G

0.25

-0.47

0.24

0.0505

-1.01

0.47

0.0302

0.03

0.23

0.8826

rs4851392

A

G

0.15

-0.75

0.36

0.0364

-1.53

0.71

0.0323

-0.04

0.25

0.8666

rs2305159

A

C

0.20

-0.59

0.28

0.0335

-1.17

0.54

0.0320

-0.15

0.23

0.5173

rs1542179

A

G

0.22

-0.62

0.27

0.0208

-1.21

0.53

0.0215

-0.21

0.23

0.3668

rs1542178

A

G

0.18

-0.65

0.32

0.0452

-1.26

0.64

0.0505

-0.24

0.24

0.3095

rs3768988

G

A

0.36

0.36

0.17

0.0389

0.50

0.33

0.1304

0.51

0.23

0.0258

rs62152925

T

C

0.22

-0.62

0.26

0.0147

-1.31

0.50

0.0092

-0.01

0.23

0.9649

rs3754680

C

T

0.23

0.81

0.34

0.0176

1.56

0.67

0.0202

0.24

0.24

0.3135

PER2

2

rs1972874

C

G

0.31

-0.33

0.19

0.0901

-0.44

0.37

0.2320

-0.47

0.23

0.0367

PER3

1

rs118049345

T

C

0.07

-2.01

0.82

0.0141

-4.02

1.63

0.0139

-0.14

0.33

0.6664

RORA

15

rs17237283

C

T

0.16

-0.83

0.37

0.0249

-1.67

0.73

0.0234

-0.10

0.24

0.6711

rs11635975

G

A

0.18

-0.56

0.28

0.0509

-1.15

0.56

0.0423

-0.04

0.24

0.8712

rs2553236

C

T

0.31

0.50

0.22

0.0221

1.09

0.42

0.0100

-0.01

0.23

0.9590

rs117194204

A

G

0.10

1.19

0.54

0.0274

2.35

1.07

0.0288

0.27

0.29

0.3524

rs13329238

C

A

0.22

-0.46

0.26

0.0744

-0.67

0.51

0.1918

-0.77

0.23

0.0009

rs8041466

T

C

0.23

-0.47

0.23

0.0454

-0.89

0.46

0.0536

-0.26

0.23

0.2606

rs12913890

G

C

0.38

-0.31

0.17

0.0722

-0.66

0.32

0.0370

-0.09

0.23

0.6889

rs7182392

T

C

0.21

-0.59

0.26

0.0252

-1.03

0.52

0.0473

-0.52

0.23

0.0251

rs4775309

A

G

0.46

-0.36

0.16

0.0258

-0.44

0.27

0.1065

-0.52

0.25

0.0385

rs11631432

C

T

0.43

0.25

0.16

0.1201

0.21

0.28

0.4515

0.48

0.24

0.0453

rs4775311

C

T

0.40

0.28

0.16

0.0818

0.28

0.29

0.3403

0.52

0.24

0.0280

rs8039990

T

C

0.32

-0.48

0.19

0.0126

-0.93

0.37

0.0114

-0.24

0.23

0.2844

rs8040450

C

G

0.34

-0.49

0.19

0.0107

-0.96

0.36

0.0084

-0.22

0.23

0.3337

rs341389

A

G

0.32

-0.48

0.19

0.0129

-0.93

0.37

0.0116

-0.24

0.23

0.2854

rs16943284

C

T

0.23

-0.15

0.24

0.5243

-0.53

0.47

0.2632

0.46

0.23

0.0444

rs7172917

T

C

0.40

0.35

0.17

0.0467

0.50

0.32

0.1145

0.41

0.23

0.0803

rs2140442

T

C

0.09

1.41

0.63

0.0261

2.92

1.27

0.0214

-0.52

0.31

0.0916

rs11638929

C

T

0.37

0.36

0.18

0.0447

0.40

0.34

0.2468

0.65

0.23

0.0044

rs17237563

T

C

0.28

0.59

0.23

0.0094

1.05

0.44

0.0184

0.44

0.22

0.0503

rs1523530

A

T

0.28

0.46

0.23

0.0497

0.81

0.45

0.0756

0.34

0.23

0.1352

rs17303341

C

T

0.24

0.67

0.25

0.0085

1.19

0.50

0.0169

0.49

0.23

0.0326

rs75336871

A

G

0.24

0.66

0.26

0.0108

1.18

0.51

0.0202

0.47

0.23

0.0401

rs4775349

C

T

0.44

-0.30

0.16

0.0554

-0.35

0.28

0.2157

-0.50

0.24

0.0341

rs17303369

T

C

0.22

0.63

0.28

0.0223

1.19

0.55

0.0302

0.33

0.23

0.1499

rs6494243

G

A

0.13

-0.17

0.33

0.6089

-0.20

0.66

0.7554

-0.55

0.26

0.0336

rs2280595

A

T

0.17

0.83

0.40

0.0381

1.63

0.79

0.0398

0.17

0.24

0.4769

rs4775350

C

T

0.23

0.64

0.27

0.0171

1.20

0.53

0.0229

0.33

0.23

0.1558

rs782948

A

G

0.12

0.001

0.37

0.9972

0.13

0.74

0.8607

-0.52

0.26

0.0495

rs12902142

C

T

0.48

0.07

0.16

0.6604

0.70

0.27

0.0102

-0.57

0.25

0.0209

rs76824799

T

C

0.15

0.90

0.41

0.0287

1.69

0.82

0.0398

0.61

0.27

0.0215

rs60094610

A

G

0.13

0.36

0.47

0.4514

0.87

0.95

0.3595

-0.53

0.25

0.0353

rs1437535

T

C

0.38

0.16

0.17

0.3488

0.72

0.32

0.0241

-0.47

0.23

0.0457

rs1437537

T

C

0.38

0.15

0.17

0.3834

0.68

0.32

0.0330

-0.45

0.23

0.0531

rs76431303

T

C

0.13

0.44

0.50

0.3815

1.04

1.01

0.3020

-0.55

0.25

0.0308

rs719006

T

A

0.32

-0.44

0.20

0.0282

-0.79

0.38

0.0383

-0.30

0.23

0.1833

rs1160694

G

A

0.32

-0.42

0.20

0.0327

-0.76

0.38

0.0441

-0.29

0.22

0.1956

rs1159814

T

C

0.31

-0.40

0.20

0.0457

-0.68

0.39

0.0762

-0.35

0.22

0.1173

rs78512626

C

A

0.11

1.17

0.54

0.0302

2.32

1.07

0.0309

0.21

0.28

0.4586

rs4238351

A

G

0.36

0.05

0.17

0.7917

-0.25

0.32

0.4405

0.51

0.23

0.0257

rs117080246

T

G

0.09

1.22

0.54

0.0229

2.40

1.07

0.0250

0.42

0.30

0.1670

rs4775368

T

C

0.42

-0.17

0.15

0.2627

-0.66

0.27

0.0156

0.27

0.24

0.2600

rs146660446

C

T

0.10

1.35

0.50

0.0072

2.73

1.00

0.0066

0.08

0.29

0.7945

RORB

9

rs1018584

A

C

0.09

-2.01

0.99

0.0427

-4.02

1.98

0.0428

-0.11

0.29

0.7117

rs1157358

T

C

0.08

-2.76

1.41

0.0504

-5.52

2.81

0.0497

0.01

0.34

0.9779

rs2273975

A

G

0.10

-1.92

0.71

0.0067

-3.78

1.41

0.0075

-0.40

0.28

0.1563

rs11144039

C

T

0.46

0.31

0.16

0.0502

0.29

0.27

0.2780

0.55

0.25

0.0280

rs72614684

T

C

0.46

0.31

0.16

0.0518

0.28

0.27

0.3074

0.56

0.25

0.0254

rs10781247

G

A

0.48

0.50

0.16

0.0021

0.32

0.27

0.2461

1.01

0.26

7.9 x 10-5

Abbreviations: A1 = minor allele, A2 = major allele, BETA = Beta coefficients, Chr = chromosome, MAF = minor allele frequency, MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination, SE = standard error.

Analysis was obtained after adjustment for covariates including age, gender, and education.

Next, we identified a nominal association of MMSE scores with 10 SNPs, including CLOCK rs3749473, NPAS2 (rs17655330, rs62152925), RORA (rs13329238, rs8040450, rs11638929, rs17237563, rs17303341, rs146660446), and RORB rs2273975 (Table 2). For further investigation in the subsequent analyses, we selected four key SNPs in four circadian clock genes with evidence of association, including CLOCK rs3749473 (P = 0.0017), NPAS2 rs17655330 (P = 0.0013), RORA rs13329238 (P = 0.0009), and RORB rs10781247 (P = 7.9 x 10-5). The genotype frequency distributions of these four SNPs were in accordance with the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium among the subjects (P = 0.400, 0.390, 0.301, and 0.378, respectively).

We then employed categorized MMSE scores as an outcome (normal: MMSE score ≥ 24; cognitive impairment: MMSE score < 24) for gene-gene and gene-environment analysis. First, generalized multifactor dimensionality reduction (GMDR) analysis was used to assess the impacts of combinations between the four key SNPs in cognitive aging, including age, gender, and education as covariates. Table 3 summarizes the results obtained from GMDR analysis for two-way up to four-way gene-gene interaction models with covariate adjustment. There were significant two-way models involving NPAS2 rs17655330 and RORB rs10781247 (P = 0.003), RORA rs13329238 and RORB rs10781247 (P = 0.003), NPAS2 rs17655330 and RORA rs13329238 (P= 0.004), and CLOCK rs3749473 and RORA rs13329238 (P = 0.042). These results indicate potential gene-gene interactions between NPAS2 and RORB, RORA and RORB, NPAS2 and RORA, and CLOCK and RORA, respectively, in influencing cognitive aging. Additionally, there were a three-way model (P = 0.001) and a four-way model (P = 0.007) indicating potential gene-gene interactions among CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB in influencing cognitive aging.

Table 3: Gene-gene interaction models identified by the GMDR method with adjustment for age, gender, and education.

Interaction model

Testing accuracy (%)

P value

CLOCK rs3749473, NPAS2 rs17655330

54.06

0.109

CLOCK rs3749473, RORA rs13329238

56.19

0.042

CLOCK rs3749473, RORB rs10781247

54.34

0.153

NPAS2 rs17655330, RORA rs13329238

58.45

0.004

NPAS2 rs17655330, RORB rs10781247

59.34

0.003

RORA rs13329238, RORB rs10781247

59.64

0.003

NPAS2 rs17655330, RORA rs13329238, RORB rs10781247

62.48

0.001

CLOCK rs3749473, NPAS2 rs17655330, RORA rs13329238, RORB rs10781247

60.82

0.007

Abbreviations: GMDR = generalized multifactor dimensionality reduction.

P value was based on 1,000 permutations. Analysis was obtained after adjustment for covariates including age, gender, and education.

Table 4 shows the GMDR analysis of gene-environment interaction models in cognitive aging using age, gender, and education as covariates. There was a significant two-way model involving CLOCK rs3749473 and environmental factors, namely smoking (P = 0.004), alcohol consumption (P = 0.01), and social support (P = 0.011), indicating potential gene-environment interactions between CLOCK and environmental factors in influencing cognitive aging. Similarly, there was a significant two-way model involving RORA rs13329238 and environmental factors such as smoking (P = 0.01), alcohol consumption (P = 0.002), physical activity (P = 0.01), and social support (P = 0.002). Finally, there was a significant two-way model involving RORB rs10781247 and the environmental factors alcohol consumption (P = 0.005) and physical activity (P = 0.035). However, there was no significant two-way model involving NPAS2 rs17655330 and environmental factors.

Table 4: Gene-environment interaction models identified by the GMDR method with adjustment for age, gender, and education.

Interaction model

Testing accuracy (%)

P value

CLOCK rs3749473, smoking

56.96

0.004

CLOCK rs3749473, alcohol consumption

56.23

0.010

CLOCK rs3749473, physical activity

53.98

0.153

CLOCK rs3749473, social support

56.71

0.011

NPAS2 rs17655330, smoking

44.48

0.979

NPAS2 rs17655330, alcohol consumption

48.78

0.668

NPAS2 rs17655330, physical activity

44.69

0.917

NPAS2 rs17655330, social support

50.48

0.444

RORA rs13329238, smoking

57.17

0.010

RORA rs13329238, alcohol consumption

59.31

0.002

RORA rs13329238, physical activity

57.95

0.010

RORA rs13329238, social support

58.16

0.002

RORB rs10781247, smoking

55.67

0.062

RORB rs10781247, alcohol consumption

58.26

0.005

RORB rs10781247, physical activity

56.93

0.035

RORB rs10781247, social support

54.31

0.135

Abbreviations: GMDR = generalized multifactor dimensionality reduction.

P value was based on 1,000 permutations. Analysis was obtained after adjustment for covariates including age, gender, and education.

Finally, Table 5 demonstrates a summarized model of the associations between the MMSE scores and SNPs within the CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB genes. Table 5 also presents a summarized model of gene-gene and gene-environment interactions among the CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB genes.

Table 5: Summarized model of the relationship between the MMSE scores and 4 selective circadian clock genes as well as their gene-gene and gene-environment interactions.

Model

P

CLOCK rs3749473 (Dominant)

0.0017

NPAS2 rs17655330 (Additive)

0.0013

NPAS2 rs17655330 (Recessive)

0.0013

NPAS2 rs62152925 (Recessive)

0.0092

RORA rs13329238 (Dominant)

0.0009

RORA rs8040450 (Recessive)

0.0084

RORA rs11638929 (Dominant)

0.0044

RORA rs17237563 (Additive)

0.0094

RORA rs17303341 (Additive)

0.0085

RORA rs146660446 (Additive)

0.0072

RORA rs146660446 (Recessive)

0.0066

RORB rs2273975 (Additive)

0.0067

RORB rs2273975 (Recessive)

0.0075

RORB rs10781247 (Additive)

0.0021

RORB rs10781247 (Dominant)

7.9 x 10-5

CLOCK rs3749473, RORA rs13329238

0.042

NPAS2 rs17655330, RORA rs13329238

0.004

NPAS2 rs17655330, RORB rs10781247

0.003

RORA rs13329238, RORB rs10781247

0.003

NPAS2 rs17655330, RORA rs13329238, RORB rs10781247

0.001

CLOCK rs3749473, NPAS2 rs17655330,

RORA rs13329238, RORB rs10781247

0.007

CLOCK rs3749473, smoking

0.004

CLOCK rs3749473, alcohol consumption

0.010

CLOCK rs3749473, social support

0.011

RORA rs13329238, smoking

0.010

RORA rs13329238, alcohol consumption

0.002

RORA rs13329238, physical activity

0.010

RORA rs13329238, social support

0.002

RORB rs10781247, alcohol consumption

0.005

RORB rs10781247, physical activity

0.035

Abbreviations: MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination.

Analysis was obtained after adjustment for covariates including age, gender, and education.

Discussion

To our knowledge, our study is the first to date to assess whether 644 SNPs in 11 circadian clock genes are significantly associated with the risk of cognitive aging independently and/or through gene-gene interactions among old Taiwanese individuals. We also examined the relationship between these genes and environmental factors to investigate whether these genes confer a risk of cognitive aging according to its effect on gene-environment interactions. Here, we report for the first time that several SNPs of the circadian clock genes, including CLOCK rs3749473, NPAS2 rs17655330, RORA rs13329238, and RORB rs10781247, may play an important role in the modulation of cognitive aging in old adults in a Taiwanese population. Notably, the association of the RORB rs10781247 SNP with MMSE scores nearly persisted after correcting for multiple testing (P < 7.8 x 10-5). Additionally, our data revealed that gene-gene interactions between CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB may contribute to the etiology of cognitive aging. Our data also indicated that there were gene-environment interactions between CLOCK, RORA, and RORB with environmental factors such as alcohol consumption, smoking status, physical activity, and social support.

To our knowledge, our results are the first to raise the possibility that 42 SNPs in the RORA gene and 6 SNPs in the RORB gene may contribute to susceptibility to cognitive aging, especially the RORA SNPs rs13329238, rs8040450, rs11638929, rs17237563, rs17303341, rs146660446, and the RORB SNPs rs10781247 and rs2273975. The proteins encoded by RORA (located on chromosome 15q22.2) and RORB (located on chromosome 9q2) constitute a subfamily of nuclear hormone receptors [3]. The RORA and RORB genes have been implicated in the regulation of a wide variety of physiological processes, including circadian rhythm, immunity, cellular metabolism, embryonic development, and inflammatory responses [17-18]. By using psychometric tests of cognitive function, Ersland et al. found that there was a strong association between the RORB gene and a test of verbal intelligence [19]. Consistent with this notion, Acquaah-Mensah et al. reported that the expression of the RORA gene is distinctly up-regulated in the hippocampi of AD-affected postmortem human brains, a brain region key to memory and learning, indicating a potential link between RORA and AD [20]. In terms of brain cognitive functions such as mood, the RORA gene was reported to increase the risk of acquiring psychiatric and neurological disorders, including bipolar disorder [6], autism spectrum disorder [21], and post-traumatic stress disorder [22]. Similarly, the RORB gene was also previously linked to psychiatric and neurological disorders, such as bipolar disorder [6, 23-24] and schizophrenia [23], in independent association studies.

In addition, an intriguing finding was a positive association of cognitive aging with 19 SNPs in the NPAS2 gene, especially rs17655330 and rs62152925. The NPAS2 gene, located on chromosome 2q11.2, encodes a member of the basic helix-loop-helix family of transcription factors. The functions of the NPAS2 and CLOCK proteins are partially redundant [25]. The ARNTL/CLOCK and ARNTL/NPAS2 heterodimeric proteins bind to chromatin, resulting in the up-regulation of CRY1, CRY2, PER1, PER2, and PER3 gene expression [5]. Based on a series of behavioral tests, Garcia et al. found that Npas2-deficient mice may have impaired brain function and that NPAS2 may activate the neuronal gene expression required for the acquisition of long-term memory, indicating its importance for the execution of complex cognitive tasks [9].

On another note, we also observed that there was an association of cognitive aging with 3 SNPs in the CLOCK gene, particularly CLOCK rs3749473. The CLOCK gene is located on chromosome 4q12 and encodes a basic helix-loop-helix protein that constitutes the ARNTL/CLOCK heterodimeric protein with ARNTL, another basic helix-loop-helix protein [26]. Evidence has also been reported for the association of three separate SNPs of the CLOCK gene (rs4580704, rs1554483, and 3111) with AD in Chinese populations [13-15]; however, these findings have not been replicated by other large AD genetics consortiums. Additionally, Kondratova et al. reported that mice with mutations in the Clock gene showed declines in cognitive performance, including increased rearing activity and impaired intersession habituation [7]. Taken together, these studies suggest the engagement of the CLOCK protein in essential processes for memory formation [7].

Remarkably, another intriguing finding was that we further inferred the epistatic effects between CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB in influencing cognitive aging by using the GMDR approach. To our knowledge, no other study has been conducted to evaluate gene-gene interactions between these genes. The functional relevance of the interactive effects between CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB on cognitive aging remains to be elucidated. At the molecular level, the circadian clock consists of an interlocking network of several positive and negative feedback loops [27]. While the positive control of the circadian oscillator in the core loop involves ARNTL, CLOCK, and NPAS2 proteins, the negative control mechanism in the core loop comprises CRY1, CRY2, PER1, PER2, and PER3 proteins [27]. Additionally, an accessory pathway involving the RORs and NR1D1 nuclear receptors further stimulates the core loop [27]. Previous animal studies characterizing Clock/Clock mutant mice demonstrated that CLOCK is not indispensable for circadian rhythms, suggesting that NPAS2, as a substitute for CLOCK, can contribute to circadian rhythms in the absence of CLOCK [25, 28]. By reporter gene and mutation analysis, Takeda et al. also suggested that the RORs nuclear receptors are involved in the circadian regulation of the transcription of circadian clock genes such as the Arntl, Cry1, Nr1d1, and Per2 genes [27]. Furthermore, Lai et al. found that there were potential interaction effects among the RORA, RORB, and NR1D1 genes associated with an increased risk of bipolar disorder by using the multifactor dimensionality reduction method [6]. These studies suggest that the possible mechanisms of joint actions between these genes may synergistically incorporate other relevant circadian clock genes.

In the GMDR analysis of gene-environment interactions, we tracked down the interplay between the CLOCK gene and environmental factors (namely smoking, alcohol consumption, and social support), between the RORA gene and environmental factors (namely smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activity, and social support), and between the RORB gene and environmental factors ( namely alcohol consumption and physical activity). This interplay may manifest itself functionally through epigenetic changes. In previous animal studies, circadian deregulation was shown to affect epigenetic parameters such as DNA methylation, histone modifications, and miRNAs, suggesting that epigenetic alterations might be linked with age-related cognitive decline [29-31]. It has also been pointed out that core circadian clock genes may activate epigenetic modifications which are involved in circadian rhythm dysfunction with advancing age [31].

In this study, we further found evidence of potential association (P < 0.05) between cognitive aging and four other genes, the ARNTL, CRY2, PER2, and PER3 genes, which are located on chromosomes 11p15, 11p11.2, 2q37.3, and 1p36.23, respectively. The ARNTL gene encodes a basic helix-loop-helix protein that forms the ARNTL/CLOCK heterodimeric protein with CLOCK [26]. The CRY2 gene encodes a flavin adenine dinucleotide-binding protein that stimulates the circadian clock [8]. The PER2 and PER3 genes are members of the Period family, which encodes components of the circadian rhythms of locomotor activity, metabolism, and feeding behavior [10]. An association of AD with the rs2278749 SNP in the ARNTL gene in a Chinese population was previously observed [16], although this biomarker has not been identified by other large AD genetics consortiums. Contrary to our findings, Pereira et al. found no association of AD with SNPs in the PER2 and PER3 genes in a Brazilian population [32]. To our knowledge, no other study has been conducted to evaluate cognitive aging with SNPs in the CRY2 gene. It is worth mentioning that multiple potential defects underly the discordant results found among these studies, including sample size, sample deviation, environmental control, study design, covariate adjustment, phenotype definitions, population stratification, and various ethnicities [33-36].

Our analysis indicated no association of cognitive aging with three other genes, the CRY1, NR1D1, and PER1 genes, which are located on chromosomes 12q23-q24.1, 17q11.2, and 17p13.1, respectively. Like the CRY2 gene, the CRY1 gene encodes a flavin adenine dinucleotide-binding protein that stimulates the circadian clock [8]. The NR1D1 gene encodes a member of the nuclear receptor subfamily 1 that negatively activates the expression of core clock proteins [6]. The PER1 gene is a member of the same Period family as the PER2 and PER3 genes, which encodes components of the circadian rhythms of locomotor activity, metabolism, and feeding behavior [10]. To our knowledge, no other study has been conducted to evaluate the association between cognitive aging and SNPs in the CRY1, NR1D1, and PER1 genes.

The MMSE was chosen to evaluate cognitive function, because it is well-established and is the most widely used screening test of cognition. Nonetheless, MMSE reduces variability in the data because it has a floor effect in the oldest adults and a ceiling effect in healthy young adults [37]. The General Practitioner Assessment of Cognition (GPCOG) was also found to have psychometric properties similar to the MMSE, but the GPCOG needs to be further assessed for its potential cultural or language bias [38, 39]. As a visual test, the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) is a computer-based cognitive assessment system that is language independent [40]. However, aspects of CANTAB still need to be validated, and only modest correlations between CANTAB and traditional neuropsychological tests such as MMSE and GPCOG have been demonstrated [41]. Additionally, a well-validated scale in cognitive performance is the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale – Cognitive section (ADAS-Cog), where a four-point change on ADAS-Cog has been established as a clinically important change in cognition [42]. Unfortunately, the length of administration time makes ADAS-Cog unsuitable for clinical practice because ADAS-Cog takes around 40 minutes to perform [37].

This study has both strengths and limitations. The main weakness is that our observations require much further research to determine whether the present research findings are sustained in diverse ethnic groups [34, 43]. To support the statistical analysis results, it is also desirable to have some additional evidence of biological functions, because the reported SNPs that influence the expression of the genes of interest have been shown to be importantly enriched for association studies [44]. In future work, prospective clinical trials with other ethnic populations would provide a comprehensive evaluation of the associations and interactions of the investigated genes with cognitive aging [35, 45, 46]. On the other hand, a major strength of our study is that we employed environmental data, which provided the opportunity to study the interplay between the investigated genes and environmental factors.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we conducted an extensive analysis of the association as well as gene-gene and gene-environment interactions of the circadian clock genes with cognitive aging in old Taiwanese subjects. Overall, results from the current study revealed that the CLOCK, NPAS2, RORA, and RORB genes may affect the prevalence of cognitive aging independently and/or through complex gene-gene and gene-environment interactions. Future independent replication studies with larger numbers of subjects will likely lead to further insights into the roles of the circadian clock genes described in this study.

Materials and Methods

Study population

This study incorporated subjects from the Taiwan Biobank [47-49]. The study cohort consisted of 634 participants. Recruitment and sample collection procedures were approved by the Internal Review Board of the Taiwan Biobank before conducting the study. Each subject signed the approved informed consent form. All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Education was defined based on whether or not high school was attended. Current alcohol drinker was defined as currently drinking 150 ml of alcohol per week for more than six months. Current smoker was defined as currently smoking for more than six months. Physical activity was defined by the amount of exercise activity exceeding three times and more than 30 minutes each time in a week. Social support was assessed based on marital status and whether or not the subject lived alone.

Cognitive assessment

Global cognitive assessment was performed using the 30-point Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), which includes questions based on the five domains of orientation, registration, attention and calculation, recall, and language. We analyzed MMSE as a continuous outcome, as well as according to categories based on previously defined MMSE thresholds [37]: MMSE score ≥ 24 (normal) and MMSE score < 24 (cognitive impairment).

Genotyping

DNA was isolated from blood samples using a QIAamp DNA blood kit following the manufacturer’s instructions (Qiagen, Valencia, CA, USA). The quality of the isolated genomic DNA was evaluated using agarose gel electrophoresis, and the quantity was determined by spectrophotometry [50, 51]. SNP genotyping was carried out using the custom Taiwan BioBank chips and run on the Axiom Genome-Wide Array Plate System (Affymetrix, Santa Clara, CA, USA). The SNP panel consisted of variants from the following 11 core circadian clock genes: ARNTL, CLOCK, CRY1, CRY2, NPAS2, NR1D1, PER1, PER2, PER3, RORA, and RORB.

Statistical analysis

In this study, we evaluated the association of the investigated SNP with MMSE scores by a general linear model using age, gender, and education as covariates [36, 33]. The genotype frequencies were assessed for Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium using a X2 goodness-of-fit test with 1 degree of freedom (i.e. the number of genotypes minus the number of alleles). In order to correct for multiple testing, we applied a conservative Bonferroni correction factor for the number of SNPs employed in the analysis. The criterion for significance was set at P < 0.05 for all tests. Data are presented as the mean ± standard deviation.

To investigate gene-gene and gene-environment interactions, we employed the generalized multifactor dimensionality reduction (GMDR) method [52]. We tested two-way up to four-way interactions using 10-fold cross-validation. The GMDR software provided some output parameters, including the testing accuracy and empirical P values, to assess each selected interaction. Moreover, we provided age, gender, and education as covariates for gene-gene and gene-environment interaction models in our interaction analyses. Permutation testing obtained empirical P values of prediction accuracy as a benchmark based on 1,000 shuffles.

Acknowledgments and Grant Support

The authors extend their sincere thanks to Vita Genomics, Inc. for funding this research. This work was supported by the Ministry of Economic Affairs in Taiwan (SBIR Grant S099000280249-154; EL), by Taipei Veterans General Hospital, Taiwan (Grants VGHUST103-G1-4-1, V105C-008, and V105E17-002-MY2-1; SJT), by the National Health Research Institutes, Taiwan (Grant NP-105-SP-04; YLL), and by the Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan (Grant MST 102-2314-B-002-117-MY3; PHK). We thank Ashley Tsai for English editing.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no potential conflicts of interests.

References

1. Ribas-Latre A, Eckel-Mahan K. Interdependence of nutrient metabolism and the circadian clock system: Importance for metabolic health. Mol Metab. 2016; 5:133-52.

2. Buhr ED, Takahashi JS. Molecular components of the Mammalian circadian clock. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2013; 217: 3–27.

3. Duez H, Staels B. Rev-erb-alpha: an integrator of circadian rhythms and metabolism. J Appl Physiol (1985). 2009; 107:1972-80.

4. Gerstner JR, Lyons LC, Wright KP Jr, Loh DH, Rawashdeh O, Eckel-Mahan KL, Roman GW. Cycling behavior and memory formation. J Neurosci. 2009; 29:12824-30.

5. Kondratova AA, Kondratov RV. The circadian clock and pathology of the ageing brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012; 13:325-35.

6. Lai YC, Kao CF, Lu ML, Chen HC, Chen PY, Chen CH, Shen WW, Wu JY, Lu RB, Kuo PH. Investigation of associations between NR1D1, RORA and RORB genes and bipolar disorder. PLoS One. 2015; 10:e0121245.

7. Kondratova AA, Dubrovsky YV, Antoch MP, Kondratov RV. Circadian clock proteins control adaptation to novel environment and memory formation. Aging (Albany NY). 2010; 2:285-97. doi: 10.18632/aging.100142.

8. Van der Zee EA, Havekes R, Barf RP, Hut RA, Nijholt IM, Jacobs EH, Gerkema MP. Circadian time-place learning in mice depends on Cry genes. Curr Biol. 2008; 18:844-8.

9. Garcia JA, Zhang D, Estill SJ, Michnoff C, Rutter J, Reick M, Scott K, Diaz-Arrastia R, McKnight SL. Impaired cued and contextual memory in NPAS2-deficient mice. Science. 2000; 288:2226-30.

10. Wang LM, Dragich JM, Kudo T, Odom IH, Welsh DK, O’Dell TJ, Colwell CS. Expression of the circadian clock gene Period2 in the hippocampus: possible implications for synaptic plasticity and learned behaviour. ASN Neuro. 2009; 1. pii: e00012.

11. Long DM, Blake MR, Dutta S, Holbrook SD, Kotwica-Rolinska J, Kretzschmar D, Giebultowicz JM. Relationships between the circadian system and Alzheimer’s disease-like symptoms in Drosophila. PLoS One. 2014; 9:e106068.

12. Musiek ES. Circadian clock disruption in neurodegenerative diseases: cause and effect? Front Pharmacol. 2015; 6:29.

13. Chen HF, Huang CQ, You C, Wang ZR, Si-qing H. Polymorphism of CLOCK gene rs 4580704 C > G is associated with susceptibility of Alzheimer’s disease in a Chinese population. Arch Med Res. 2013; 44:203-7.

14. Chen Q, Huang CQ, Hu XY, Li SB, Zhang XM. Functional CLOCK gene rs1554483 G/C polymorphism is associated with susceptibility to Alzheimer’s disease in the Chinese population. J Int Med Res. 2013; 41:340-6.

15. Yang YK, Peng XD, Li YH, Wang ZR, Chang-quan H, Hui W, Liu QX. The polymorphism of CLOCK gene 3111T/C C>T is associated with susceptibility of Alzheimer disease in Chinese population. J Investig Med. 2013; 61:1084-7.

16. Chen Q, Peng XD, Huang CQ, Hu XY, Zhang XM. Association between ARNTL (BMAL1) rs2278749 polymorphism T >C and susceptibility to Alzheimer disease in a Chinese population. Genet Mol Res. 2015; 14:18515-22.

17. Kang HS, Okamoto K, Takeda Y, Beak JY, Gerrish K, Bortner CD, DeGraff LM, Wada T, Xie W, Jetten AM. Transcriptional profiling reveals a role for RORalpha in regulating gene expression in obesity-associated inflammation and hepatic steatosis. Physiol Genomics. 2011; 43:818-28.

18. Jetten AM. Retinoid-related orphan receptors (RORs): critical roles in development, immunity, circadian rhythm, and cellular metabolism. Nucl Recept Signal. 2009; 7:e003.

19. Ersland KM, Christoforou A, Stansberg C, Espeseth T, Mattheisen M, Mattingsdal M, Hardarson GA, Hansen T, Fernandes CP, Giddaluru S, Breuer R, Strohmaier J, Djurovic S, et al. Gene-based analysis of regionally enriched cortical genes in GWAS data sets of cognitive traits and psychiatric disorders. PLoS One. 2012; 7:e31687.

20. Acquaah-Mensah GK, Agu N, Khan T, Gardner A. A regulatory role for the insulin- and BDNF-linked RORA in the hippocampus: implications for Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015; 44:827-38.

21. Nguyen A, Rauch TA, Pfeifer GP, Hu VW. Global methylation profiling of lymphoblastoid cell lines reveals epigenetic contributions to autism spectrum disorders and a novel autism candidate gene, RORA, whose protein product is reduced in autistic brain. FASEB J. 2010; 24:3036-51.

22. Logue MW, Baldwin C, Guffanti G, Melista E, Wolf EJ, Reardon AF, Uddin M, Wildman D, Galea S, Koenen KC, Miller MW. A genome-wide association study of post-traumatic stress disorder identifies the retinoid-related orphan receptor alpha (RORA) gene as a significant risk locus. Mol Psychiatry. 2013; 18:937-42.

23. Mansour HA, Talkowski ME, Wood J, Chowdari KV, McClain L, Prasad K, Montrose D, Fagiolini A, Friedman ES, Allen MH, Bowden CL, Calabrese J, El-Mallakh RS, et al. Association study of 21 circadian genes with bipolar I disorder, schizoaffective disorder, and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord. 2009; 11:701-10.

24. McGrath CL, Glatt SJ, Sklar P, Le-Niculescu H, Kuczenski R, Doyle AE, Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV, Niculescu AB, Tsuang MT. Evidence for genetic association of RORB with bipolar disorder. BMC Psychiatry. 2009; 9:70.

25. DeBruyne JP, Weaver DR, Reppert SM. CLOCK and NPAS2 have overlapping roles in the suprachiasmatic circadian clock. Nat Neurosci. 2007; 10:543-5.

26. Woon PY, Kaisaki PJ, Bragança J, Bihoreau MT, Levy JC, Farrall M, Gauguier D. Aryl hydrocarbon receptor nuclear translocator-like (BMAL1) is associated with susceptibility to hypertension and type 2 diabetes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007; 104:14412-7.

27. Takeda Y, Jothi R, Birault V, Jetten AM. RORγ directly regulates the circadian expression of clock genes and downstream targets in vivo. Nucleic Acids Res. 2012; 40:8519-35.

28. Landgraf D, Wang LL, Diemer T, Welsh DK. NPAS2 Compensates for Loss of CLOCK in Peripheral Circadian Oscillators. PLoS Genet. 2016; 12:e1005882.

29. Kochan DZ, Ilnytskyy Y, Golubov A, Deibel SH, McDonald RJ, Kovalchuk O. Circadian disruption-induced microRNAome deregulation in rat mammary gland tissues. Oncoscience. 2015; 2:428-42. doi: 10.18632/oncoscience.157.

30. Kochan DZ, Ilnytskyy Y, Golubov A, Deibel SH, McDonald RJ, Kovalchuk O. Circadian-disruption-induced gene expression changes in rodent mammary tissues. Oncoscience. 2016; 3:58-70. doi: 10.18632/oncoscience.292.

31. Deibel SH, Zelinski EL, Keeley RJ, Kovalchuk O, McDonald RJ. Epigenetic alterations in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and hippocampus contribute to age-related cognitive decline. Oncotarget. 2015; 6:23181-203. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.4036.

32. Pereira PA, Alvim-Soares A, Bicalho MA, Moraes EN, Malloy-Diniz L, Paula JJ, Romano-Silva MA, Miranda DM. Lack of Association between Genetic Polymorphism of Circadian Genes (PER2, PER3, CLOCK and OX2R) with Late Onset Depression and Alzheimer’s Disease in a Sample of a Brazilian Population (Circadian Genes, Late-Onset Depression and Alzheimer’s Disease). Curr Alzheimer Res. 2016; 13:1397-1406.

33. Hsiao TJ, Hwang Y, Liu CH, Chang HM, Lin E. Association of the C825T polymorphism in the GNB3 gene with obesity and metabolic phenotypes in a Taiwanese population. Genes Nutr. 2013; 8:137-44.

34. Hsiao TJ, Hwang Y, Chang HM, Lin E. Association of the rs6235 variant in the proprotein convertase subtilisin/kexin type 1 (PCSK1) gene with obesity and related traits in a Taiwanese population. Gene. 2014; 533:32-7.

35. Hsiao TJ, Lin E. Evaluation of the glutamine 27 glutamic acid polymorphism in the adrenoceptor beta 2 surface gene on obesity and metabolic phenotypes in Taiwan. J Investig Med. 2014; 62:310-5.

36. Hsiao TJ, Lin E. The Pro12Ala polymorphism in the peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma (PPARG) gene in relation to obesity and metabolic phenotypes in a Taiwanese population. Endocrine. 2015; 48:786-93.

37. Sheehan B. Assessment scales in dementia. Ther Adv Neurol Disord. 2012; 5:349-58.

38. Brodaty H, Low LF, Gibson L, Burns K. What is the best dementia screening instrument for general practitioners to use? Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006; 14:391-400.

39. Milne A, Culverwell A, Guss R, Tuppen J, Whelton R. Screening for dementia in primary care: a review of the use, efficacy and quality of measures. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008; 20:911-26.

40. Wild K, Howieson D, Webbe F, Seelye A, Kaye J. Status of computerized cognitive testing in aging: a systematic review. Alzheimers Dement. 2008; 4:428-37.

41. Smith PJ, Need AC, Cirulli ET, Chiba-Falek O, Attix DK. A comparison of the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) with “traditional” neuropsychological testing instruments. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2013; 35:319-28.

42. Rockwood K, Fay S, Gorman M, Carver D, Graham JE. The clinical meaningfulness of ADAS-Cog changes in Alzheimer’s disease patients treated with donepezil in an open-label trial. BMC Neurol. 2007; 7:26.

43. Hsiao TJ, Lin E. A common rs7903146 variant of the transcription factor 7 like 2 gene is associated with type 2 diabetes mellitus and fasting glucose in a Taiwanese population. Diabetes Metab. 2017; 43:83-85. doi: 10.1016/j.diabet.2016.05.003.

44. Schaub MA, Boyle AP, Kundaje A, Batzoglou S, Snyder M. Linking disease associations with regulatory information in the human genome. Genome Res. 2012; 22:1748-59.

45. Lane HY, Tsai GE, Lin E. Assessing gene-gene interactions in pharmacogenomics. Mol Diagn Ther. 2012; 16:15-27.

46. Hsiao TJ, Lin E. A validation study of adiponectin rs266729 gene variant with type 2 diabetes, obesity, and metabolic phenotypes in a Taiwanese population. Biochem Genet. 2016; 54:830-841.

47. Fan CT, Lin JC, Lee CH. Taiwan Biobank: a project aiming to aid Taiwan’s transition into a biomedical island. Pharmacogenomics. 2008; 9:235-46.

48. Lin E, Kuo PH, Liu YL, Yang AC, Kao CF, Tsai SJ. Association and interaction of APOA5, BUD13, CETP, LIPA and health-related behavior with metabolic syndrome in a Taiwanese population. Sci Rep. 2016; 6:36830.

49. Lin E, Tsai SJ, Kuo PH, Liu YL, Yang AC, Kao CF, Yang CH. The rs1277306 variant of the REST gene confers susceptibility to cognitive aging in the elderly in a Taiwanese population. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2017; 43:119-127.

50. Liou YJ, Bai YM, Lin E, Chen JY, Chen TT, Hong CJ, Tsai SJ. Gene-gene interactions of the INSIG1 and INSIG2 in metabolic syndrome in schizophrenic patients treated with atypical antipsychotics. Pharmacogenomics J. 2012; 12:54-61

51. Hsiao TJ, Lin E. The ENPP1 K121Q polymorphism is associated with type 2 diabetes and related metabolic phenotypes in a Taiwanese population. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2016; 433:20-25.

52. Lou XY, Chen GB, Yan L, Ma JZ, Zhu J, Elston RC, Li MD. A generalized combinatorial approach for detecting gene-by-gene and gene-by-environment interactions with application to nicotine dependence. Am J Hum Genet. 2007; 80:1125–1137.


Creative Commons License All site content, except where otherwise noted, is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License.
PII: 15493